Ian Stevenson (October 31, 1918 ~ February
8, 2007) was the former head of the Department of Psychiatry at the
University of Virginia, and now is Director of the Division of Perceptual
Studies at the University of Virginia. He has devoted the last 40 years to the
scientific documentation of past life memories of children from all over the
world and has over 3000 cases in his files. Many people, including skeptics and
scholars, agree that these cases offer the best evidence yet for reincarnation.
.
.
Dr. Stevenson's research into the possibility of reincarnation began in 1960 when he heard of a case in Sri Lanka where a child claimed to remember a past life. He thoroughly questioned the child and the child's parents, as well as the people whom the child claimed were his parents from his past life. This led to Dr. Stevenson's conviction that reincarnation was possibly a reality. The more cases he pursued, the greater became his drive to scientifically open up and conquer an unknown territory among the world's mysteries, which until now had been excluded from scientific observation. Nonetheless, he believed he could approach and possibly furnish proof of its reality with scientific means.
In 1960, Dr. Stevenson published two
articles in the Journal
of the American Society for Psychical Research about children who
remembered past lives. In 1974, he published his book, Twenty Cases Suggestive of Reincarnation, and became well
known wherever this book appeared by those people who already had a
long-standing interest in this subject.
They were pleased to finally be
presented with such fundamental research into reincarnation from a scientific
source. In 1997, Dr. Stevenson published his work entitled Reincarnation and Biology. In the first
volume, he mainly describes birthmarks ~ those distinguishing marks on the skin
which the newborn baby brings into the world and cannot be explained by
inheritance alone.
In his second volume, Dr. Stevenson
focuses mainly on deformities and other anomalies that children are born with
and which cannot be traced back to inheritance, prenatal or perinatal (created
during birth) occurrences. This monumental piece of work contains hundreds of
pictures documenting the evidence.
During his original research into
various cases involving children's memories of past lives, Dr. Stevenson did
note with interest the fact that these children frequently bore lasting
birthmarks which supposedly related to their murder or the death they suffered
in a previous life.
Stevenson's research into birthmarks
and congenital defects has such particular importance for the demonstration of
reincarnation, since it furnishes objective and graphic proof of reincarnation,
superior to the ~ often fragmentary ~ memories and reports of the children and
adults questioned, which, even if verified afterwards, cannot be assigned the
same value in scientific terms.
In many cases presented by Dr.
Stevenson there are also medical documents available as further proof, which
are usually compiled after the death of the person. Dr. Stevenson adds that in
the cases he researched and "solved" in which birthmarks and
deformities were present, he didn't suppose there was any other apposite explanation
than that of reincarnation.
Only 30% ~ 60% of these deformities
can be put down to birth defects which related to genetic factors, virus
infections or chemical causes (like those found in children damaged by the drug
Thalidomide or alcohol). Apart from these demonstrable causes, the medical
profession has no other explanation for the other 40% to 70% of cases than that
of mere chance. Stevenson has now succeeded in giving us an explanation of why
a person is born with these deformities and why they appear precisely in that
part of their body and not in another.
Most of the cases where birthmarks
and congenital deformities are present for which no medical explanations exist
have one to five characteristics in common.
(1)In the most unusual scenario, it is possible that someone who believed in reincarnation expressed a wish to be reborn to a couple or one partner of a couple. This is usually because they are convinced that they would be well cared for by those particular people. Such preliminary requests are often expressed by the Tlingit Indians of Alaska and by the Tibetans.(2)More frequent than this are the occurrences of prophetic dreams. Someone who has died appears to a pregnant or not as yet pregnant woman and tells her that he or she will be reborn to her. Sometimes relatives or friends have dreams like this and will then relate the dream to the mother to be. Dr. Stevenson found these prophetic dreams to be particularly prolific in Burma and among the Indians in Alaska.(3)In these cultures the body of a newborn child is checked for recognizable marks to establish whether the deceased person they had once known has been reborn to them. This searching for marks of identification is very common among cultures that believe in reincarnation, and especially among the Tlingit Indians and the Igbos of Nigeria. Various tribes of West Africa make marks on the body of the recently deceased in order to be able to identify the person when he or she is reborn.(4)The most frequently occurring event or common denominator relating to rebirth is probably that of a child remembering a past life. Children usually begin to talk about their memories between the ages of two and four. Such infantile memories gradually dwindle when the child is between four and seven years old. There are of course always some exceptions, such as a child continuing to remember its previous life but not speaking about it for various reasons.Most of the children talk about their previous identity with great intensity and feeling. Often they cannot decide for themselves which world is real and which one is not. They often experience a kind of double existence where at times one life is more prominent, and at times the other life takes over. This is why they usually speak of their past life in the present tense saying things like, "I have a husband and two children who live in Jaipur." Almost all of them are able to tell us about the events leading up to their death.Such children tend to consider their previous parents to be their real parents rather than their present ones, and usually express a wish to return to them. When the previous family has been found and details about the person in that past life have come to light, then the origin of the fifth common denominator ~ the conspicuous or unusual behavior of the child ~ is becoming obvious.(5)For instance, if the child is born in India to a very low-class family and was a member of a higher caste in its previous life, it may feel uncomfortable in its new family. The child may ask to be served or waited on hand and foot and may refuse to wear cheap clothes. Stevenson gives us several examples of these unusual behavior patterns.In 35% of cases he investigated, children who died an unnatural death developed phobias. For example, if they had drowned in a past life then they frequently developed a phobia about going out of their depth in water. If they had been shot, they were often afraid of guns and sometimes loud bangs in general. If they died in a road accident they would sometimes develop a phobia of traveling in cars, buses or lorries.Another frequently observed unusual form of behavior, which Dr. Stevenson called philias, concerns children who express the wish to eat different kinds of food or to wear clothes that were different from those of their culture. If a child had developed an alcohol, tobacco or drug addiction as an adult in a previous incarnation he may express a need for these substances and develop cravings at an early age.Many of these children with past-life memories show abilities or talents that they had in their previous lives. Often children who were members of the opposite sex in their previous life show difficulty in adjusting to the new sex. These problems relating to the 'sex change' can lead to homosexuality later on in their lives. Former girls who were reborn as boys may wish to dress as girls or prefer to play with girls rather than boys.
Until now all these human oddities
have been a mystery to conventional psychiatrists ~ after all, the parents
could not be blamed for their children's behavior in these cases. At long last
research into reincarnation is shedding some light on the subject. In the past,
doctors blamed such peculiarities on a lack or a surplus of certain hormones,
but now they will have to do some rethinking.
The following paper by Dr. Stevenson
was presented at the Eleventh Annual Meeting of the Society
for Scientific Exploration held at Princeton University. June
11-13, 1992. The title of the paper is "Birthmarks and Birth Defects
Corresponding to Wounds on Deceased Persons" and provides perhaps the most
compelling scientific evidence suggestive of reincarnation. Dr. Stevenson's
paper presents evidence that physical characteristics, such as birthmarks and
deformities, may be carried over from a past life to a present life.
BIRTHMARKS AND BIRTH DEFECTS
CORRESPONDING TO WOUNDS ON DECEASED PERSONS
ABSTRACT
Ian Stevenson, Department of Psychiatric Medicine, University of Virginia, School of Medicine, Charlottesville, Virginia 22908
Almost nothing is known about why pigmented birthmarks (moles or nevi) occur in particular locations of the skin. The causes of most birth defects are also unknown. About 35% of children who claim to remember previous lives have birthmarks and/or birth defects that they (or adult informants) attribute to wounds on a person whose life the child remembers.
Ian Stevenson, Department of Psychiatric Medicine, University of Virginia, School of Medicine, Charlottesville, Virginia 22908
Almost nothing is known about why pigmented birthmarks (moles or nevi) occur in particular locations of the skin. The causes of most birth defects are also unknown. About 35% of children who claim to remember previous lives have birthmarks and/or birth defects that they (or adult informants) attribute to wounds on a person whose life the child remembers.
The cases of 210 such children have
been investigated. The birthmarks were usually areas of hairless, puckered
skin; some were areas of little or no pigmentation (hypopigmented macules);
others were areas of increased pigmentation (hyperpigmented nevi).
The birth defects were nearly always
of rare types. In cases in which a deceased person was identified the details
of whose life unmistakably matched the child's statements, a close
correspondence was nearly always found between the birthmarks and/or birth
defects on the child and the wounds on the deceased person.
In 43 of 49 cases in which a medical
document (usually a postmortem report) was obtained, it confirmed the
correspondence between wounds and birthmarks (or birth defects). There is
little evidence that parents and other informants imposed a false identity on
the child in order to explain the child's birthmark or birth defect. Some
paranormal process seems required to account for at least some of the details
of these cases, including the birthmarks and birth defects.
INTRODUCTION
Although counts of moles (hyperpigmented nevi) have shown that the average adult has between 15 and IX of them (Pack and Davis, 1956), little is known about their cause ~ except for those associated with the genetic disease neurofibromatosis ~ and even less is known about why birthmarks occur in one location of the body instead of in another.
Although counts of moles (hyperpigmented nevi) have shown that the average adult has between 15 and IX of them (Pack and Davis, 1956), little is known about their cause ~ except for those associated with the genetic disease neurofibromatosis ~ and even less is known about why birthmarks occur in one location of the body instead of in another.
In a few instances a genetic factor has been
plausibly suggested for the location of nevi (Cockayne, 1933; Denaro, 1944;
Maruri, 1961); but the cause of the location of most birthmarks remains
unknown. The causes of many, perhaps most, birth defects remain similarly
unknown.
In large series of birth defects in
which investigators have searched for the known causes, such as chemical
teratogens (like thalidomide), viral infections, and genetic factors, between
430/0 (Nelson and Holmes, 1989) and 65 ~ 70% (Wilson, 1973) of cases have
finally been assigned to the category of "unknown causes."
Among 895 cases of children who claimed to remember a previous life (or were thought by adults to have had a previous life), birthmarks and/or birth defects attributed to the previous life were reported in 309 (35%) of the subjects. The birthmark or birth defect of the child was said to correspond to a wound (usually fatal) or other mark on the deceased person whose life the child said it remembered. This paper reports an inquiry into the validity of such claims. With my associates I have now carried the investigation of 210 such cases to a stage where I can report their details in a forthcoming book (Stevenson, forthcoming). This article summarizes our findings.
Children who claim to remember previous lives have been found in every part of the world where they have been looked for (Stevenson, 1983; 1987), but they are found most easily in the countries of South Asia. Typically, such a child begins to speak about a previous life almost as soon as it can speak, usually between the ages of two and three; and typically it stops doing so between the ages of five and seven (Cook, Pasricha, Samararatne, Win Maung, and Stevenson, 1983).
Among 895 cases of children who claimed to remember a previous life (or were thought by adults to have had a previous life), birthmarks and/or birth defects attributed to the previous life were reported in 309 (35%) of the subjects. The birthmark or birth defect of the child was said to correspond to a wound (usually fatal) or other mark on the deceased person whose life the child said it remembered. This paper reports an inquiry into the validity of such claims. With my associates I have now carried the investigation of 210 such cases to a stage where I can report their details in a forthcoming book (Stevenson, forthcoming). This article summarizes our findings.
Children who claim to remember previous lives have been found in every part of the world where they have been looked for (Stevenson, 1983; 1987), but they are found most easily in the countries of South Asia. Typically, such a child begins to speak about a previous life almost as soon as it can speak, usually between the ages of two and three; and typically it stops doing so between the ages of five and seven (Cook, Pasricha, Samararatne, Win Maung, and Stevenson, 1983).
Although some of the children make
only vague statements, others give details of names and events that permit
identifying a person whose life and death corresponds to the child's
statements. In some instances the person identified is already known to the
child's family, but in many cases this is not so. In addition to making
verifiable statements about a deceased person, many of the children show
behavior (such as a phobia) that is unusual in their family but found to
correspond to behavior shown by the deceased person concerned or conjecturable
for him (Stevenson, 1987; 1990).
Although some of the birthmarks
occurring on these children are "ordinary" hyperpigmented nevi
(moles) of which every adult has some (Pack and Davis, 1956), most are not.
Instead, they are more likely to be puckered and scarlike, sometimes depressed
a little below the surrounding skin, areas of hairlessness, areas of markedly
diminished pigmentation (hypopigmented macules), or port-wine stains
(nevipammri). When a relevant birthmark is a hyperpigmented nevus, it is nearly
always larger in area than the "ordinary" hyperpigmented nevus.
Similarly, the birth defects in these cases are of unusual types and rarely
correspond to any of the "recognizable patterns of human
malformation" (Smith, 1982).
METHODS
My investigations of these cases included interviews, often repeated, with the subject and with several or many other informants for both families. With rare exceptions, only firsthand informants were interviewed.
METHODS
My investigations of these cases included interviews, often repeated, with the subject and with several or many other informants for both families. With rare exceptions, only firsthand informants were interviewed.
All pertinent written records that
existed, particularly death certificates and postmortem reports, were sought
and examined. In the cases in which the informants said that the two families
had no previous acquaintance, I made every effort to exclude all possibility
that some information might nevertheless have passed normally to the child,
perhaps through a half-forgotten mutual acquaintance of the two families. I
have published elsewhere full details about methods (Stevenson, 1975; 1987).
I did not accept any indicated mark as a birthmark unless a firsthand witness assured me that it had been noticed immediately after the child's birth or, at most, within a few weeks. I enquired about the occurrence of similar birth marks in other members of the family; in nearly every instance this was denied, but in seven cases a genetic factor could not be excluded.
Birth defects of the kind in question here would be noticed immediately after the child's birth. Inquiries in these cases excluded (again with rare exceptions) the known causes of birth defects, such as close biological relationship of the parents (consanguinity), viral infections in the subject's mother during her pregnancy, and chemical causes of birth defects like alcohol.
I did not accept any indicated mark as a birthmark unless a firsthand witness assured me that it had been noticed immediately after the child's birth or, at most, within a few weeks. I enquired about the occurrence of similar birth marks in other members of the family; in nearly every instance this was denied, but in seven cases a genetic factor could not be excluded.
Birth defects of the kind in question here would be noticed immediately after the child's birth. Inquiries in these cases excluded (again with rare exceptions) the known causes of birth defects, such as close biological relationship of the parents (consanguinity), viral infections in the subject's mother during her pregnancy, and chemical causes of birth defects like alcohol.
RESULTS
CORRESPONDENCES
BETWEEN WOUNDS AND BIRTHMARKS
A correspondence between birthmark
and wound was judged satisfactory if the birthmark and wound were both within
an area of 10 square centimeters at the same anatomical location; in fact, many
of the birthmarks and wounds were much closer to the same location than this.
A medical document, usually a
postmortem report, was obtained in 49 cases. The correspondence between wound
and birthmark was judged satisfactory or better by the mentioned criterion in
43 (88%) of these cases and not satisfactory in 6 cases. Several different
explanations seem to be required to account for the discrepant cases, and I
discuss these elsewhere (Stevenson. forthcoming).
Figure 1 shows a birthmark (an urea of hypopigmentation) on an Indian child who said he remembered the life of a man, Maha Ram, who had been killed with a shotgun fired at close range.
Figure 2. The circles show the principal shotgun wounds on Maha
Ram, for comparison with Figure 1. This drawing is from the autopsy report of
the deceased and shows the location of the wounds recorded by the pathologist.
(The circles were drawn by an Indian physician who studied the postmortem
report with me.)
The high proportion (88%) of concordance between wounds and birthmarks in the cases for which we obtained postmortem reports (or other confirming documents) increases confidence in the accuracy of informants' memories concerning the wounds on the deceased person in those more numerous cases for which we could obtain no medical document.
Not all errors of informants’
memories would have resulted in attributing a correspondence between birth
marks and wounds that did not exist; in four cases (possibly five) reliance on
an informant's memory would have resulted in missing a correspondence to which
a medical document attested.
CASES WITH
TWO OR MORE BIRTHMARKS
The argument of chance as accounting
for the correspondence between birthmarks and wounds becomes much reduced when
the child has two or more birthmarks each corresponding to a wound on the
deceased person whose life he claims to remember.
.
Figure 3 shows a major abnormality of the skin (verrucous epidermal nevus) on the back of the head of a Thai man who, as a child, recalled the life of his uncle, who had been struck on the head with a heavy knife and killed almost instantly.
Figure 4.
Congenital malformation of nail on right great toe of the Thai subject shown in
Figure 3. This malformation corresponded to a chronic ulcer of the right great
toe from which the subject's uncle had suffered for some
years before he died.
The series includes 18 cases in which two birthmarks on a subject corresponded to gunshot wounds of entry and exit. In 14 of these one birthmark was larger than the other, and in 9 of these 14 the evidence clearly showed that the smaller birthmark (usually round) corresponded to the wound of entry and the larger one (usually irregular in shape) corresponded to the wound of exit. These observations accord with the fact that bullet wounds of exit are nearly always larger than wounds of entry (Fatteh, 1976; Gordon and Shapiro, 1982).
Figure 5 shows a small round birthmark on the back of the head of a Thai boy, and Figure 6 shows a larger, irregularly shaped birthmark at the front of his head. The boy said that he remembered the life of a man who was shot in the head from behind. (The mode of death was verified, but no medical document was obtainable.)
In addition to the 9 cases I have
investigated myself, Mills reported another case having the feature of a small
round birthmark (corresponding to the wound of entry) and a larger birthmark
corresponding to the wound of exit (both verified by a postmortem report)
(Mills, 1989).
I have calculated the odds against chance of two birthmarks correctly corresponding to two wounds. The surface area of the skin of the average adult male is 1.6 meters (Spalteholz, 1943). If we were to imagine this area square and spread on a flat surface, its dimensions would be approximately 127 centimeters by 127 centimeters. Into this area would fit approximately 160 squares of the size 10 centimeters square that I mentioned above.
I have calculated the odds against chance of two birthmarks correctly corresponding to two wounds. The surface area of the skin of the average adult male is 1.6 meters (Spalteholz, 1943). If we were to imagine this area square and spread on a flat surface, its dimensions would be approximately 127 centimeters by 127 centimeters. Into this area would fit approximately 160 squares of the size 10 centimeters square that I mentioned above.
The probability that a single
birthmark on a person would correspond in location to a wound within the area
of any of the 160 smaller squares is only 1/160. However, the probability of
correspondences between two birthmarks and two wounds would be (1/160)2 i.e. 1
in 25,600. (This calculation assumes that birthmarks are uniformly distributed
over all regions of the skin. This is incorrect [Pack, Lenson, and Gerber,
1952], but I believe the variation can be ignored for the present purpose.)
EXAMPLES OF
OTHER CORRESPONDENCES OF DETAIL
BETWEEN WOUNDS AND BIRTHMARKS
BETWEEN WOUNDS AND BIRTHMARKS
A Thai woman had three separate
linear hypopigmented scarlike birthmarks near the midline of her back; as a
child she had remembered the life of a woman who was killed when struck three
times in the back with an ax. (Informants verified this mode of death, but no
medical record was obtainable.)
A woman of Burma was born with two
perfectly round birthmarks in her left chest; they slightly overlapped, and one
was about half the size of the other. As a child she said that she remembered
the life of a woman who was accidentally shot and killed with a shotgun. A
responsible informant said the shotgun cartridge had contained shot of two
different sizes. (No medical record was obtainable in this case.)
Another Burmese child said that she remembered the life of her deceased aunt, who had died during surgery for congenital heart disease. This child had a long, vertical linear hypopigmented birthmark close to the midline of her lower chest and upper abdomen; this birthmark corresponded to the surgical incision for the repair of the aunt's heart. (I obtained a medical record in this case.)
Another Burmese child said that she remembered the life of her deceased aunt, who had died during surgery for congenital heart disease. This child had a long, vertical linear hypopigmented birthmark close to the midline of her lower chest and upper abdomen; this birthmark corresponded to the surgical incision for the repair of the aunt's heart. (I obtained a medical record in this case.)
In contrast, a child of Turkey had a
horizontal linear birthmark across the right upper quadrant of his abdomen. It
resembled the scar of a surgeon's transverse abdominal incision. The child said
that he remembered the life of his paternal grandfather, who had become
jaundiced and was operated on before he died. He may have had a cancer of the
head of the pancreas, but I could not learn a precise medical diagnosis.
Two Burmese subjects remembered as children the lives of persons who had died after being bitten by venomous snakes, and the birthmarks of each corresponded to therapeutic incisions made at the sites of the snakebites on the persons whose lives they remembered.
Two Burmese subjects remembered as children the lives of persons who had died after being bitten by venomous snakes, and the birthmarks of each corresponded to therapeutic incisions made at the sites of the snakebites on the persons whose lives they remembered.
Another Burmese subject also said as
a child that she remembered the life of a child who had been bitten on the foot
by a snake and died. In this case, however, the child's uncle had applied a
burning cheroot to the site of the bite ~ a folk remedy for snakebite in parts
of Burma; and the subject's birthmark was round and located at the site on the
foot where the bitten child's uncle had applied the cheroot.
THREE EXAMPLES OF BIRTH DEFECTS
Figure 8, shows the right side of the head of a Turkish boy with a diminished and malformed ear (unilateral microtia). He also had underdevelopment of the right side of his face (hemifacial microsomia). He said that he remembered the life of a man who had been shot (with a shotgun) at point-blank range. The wounded man was taken to a hospital where he died 6 days later ~ of injuries to the brain caused by shot that had penetrated the right side of the skull. (I obtained a copy of the hospital record.)
THREE EXAMPLES OF BIRTH DEFECTS
Figure 8, shows the right side of the head of a Turkish boy with a diminished and malformed ear (unilateral microtia). He also had underdevelopment of the right side of his face (hemifacial microsomia). He said that he remembered the life of a man who had been shot (with a shotgun) at point-blank range. The wounded man was taken to a hospital where he died 6 days later ~ of injuries to the brain caused by shot that had penetrated the right side of the skull. (I obtained a copy of the hospital record.)
Figure 9 shows fingers almost absent congenitally on one hand (unilateral brachydactyly) in a child of India who said he remembered the life of another child who had put his right hand into the blades of a fodder-chopping machine and lost his fingers. Most cases of brachydactyly involve only a shortening of the middle phalanges. In the present case there were no phalangeal bones, and the fingers were represented by mere stubs. Unilateral brachydactyly is exceedingly rare, and I have not found a published report of a case, although a colleague (plastic surgeon) has shown me a photograph of one case that came under his care.
Figure 10 shows congenital absence of the lower right leg (unilateral hemimelia) in a Burmese girl. She said that she remembered the life of a girl who was run over by a train. Eyewitnesses said that the train severed the girl's right leg first, before running over the trunk. Lower hemimelia is an extremely rare condition, and Frantz and O'Rahilly (1961) found it in only 12 (4.0%) of 300 cases of all congenital skeletal deficiencies that they examined.
DISCUSSION
Because most (but not all) of these cases develop among persons who believe in reincarnation, we should expect that the informants for the cases would interpret them as examples according with their belief; and they usually do. It is necessary, however, for scientists to think of alternative explanations.
The most obvious explanation of these cases attributes the birthmark or birth defect on the child to chance, and the reports of the child's statements and unusual behavior then become a parental fiction intended to account for the birthmark (or birth defect) in terms of the culturally accepted belief in reincarnation.
There are, however, important
objections to this explanation.
.
.
FIRST,
the parents (and other adults concerned in a case) have no need to invent and narrate details of a previous life in order to explain their child's lesion. Believing in reincarnation, as most of them do, they are nearly always content to attribute the lesion to some event of a previous life without searching for a particular life with matching details.
SECOND,
the lives of the deceased persons figuring in the cases were of uneven quality both as to social status and commendable conduct. A few of them provided models of heroism or some other enviable quality; but many of them lived in poverty or were otherwise unexemplary. Few parents would impose an identification with such persons on their children.
THIRD,
although in most cases the two families concerned were acquainted (or even related), I am confident that in at least 13 cases (among 210 carefully examined with regard to this matter) the two families concerned had never even heard about each other before the case developed. The subject's family in these cases can have had no information with which to build up an imaginary previous life which, it later turned out, closely matched a real one.
In another 12 cases the child's parents had heard about the death of the person concerned, but had no knowledge of the wounds on that person. Limitations of space for this article oblige me to ask readers to accept my appraisal of these 25 cases for this matter; but in my forthcoming work I give a list of the cases from which readers can find the detailed reports of the cases and from reading them judge this important question for themselves.
FOURTH,
I think I have shown that chance is an improbable interpretation for the correspondences in location between two or more birthmarks on the subject of a case and wounds on a deceased person.
Persons who reject the explanation of chance combined with a secondarily confected history may consider other interpretations that include paranormal processes, but fall short of proposing a life after death.
One of these supposes that the birthmark or birth defect occurs by chance and the subject then by telepathy learns about a deceased person who had a similar lesion and develops an identification with that person. The children subjects of these cases, however, never show paranormal powers of the magnitude required to explain the apparent memories in contexts outside of their seeming memories.Another explanation, which would leave less to chance in the production of the child's lesion, attributes it to a maternal impression on the part of the child's mother. According to this idea, a pregnant woman, having a knowledge of the deceased person's wounds, might influence a gestating embryo and fetus so that its form corresponded to the wounds on the deceased person.
The idea of maternal impressions,
popular in preceding centuries and up to the first decades of this one, has
fallen into disrepute. Until my own recent article (Stevenson, 1992) there had
been no review of series of cases since 1890 (Dabney, 1890); and cases are
rarely published now (Williams and Pembroke, 1988).
Nevertheless, some of the published
cases ~ old and new ~ show a remarkable correspondence between an unusual
stimulus in the mind of a pregnant woman and an unusual birthmark or birth
defect in her later-born child.
Also, in an analysis of 113 published
cases I found that the stimulus occurred to the mother in the first trimester
in 80 cases (Stevenson, 1992). The first trimester is well known to be the one
of greatest sensitivity of the embryo/fetus to recognized teratogens, such as
thalidomide (Nowack, 1965) and rubella (Hill, Doll, Galloway, and Hughes,
1958).
Applied to the present cases,
however, the theory of maternal impression has obstacles as great as the normal
explanation appears to have.
.
.
FIRST,
in the 25 cases mentioned above, the subject's mother, although she may have heard of the death of the concerned deceased person, had no knowledge of that person's wounds.
SECOND,
this interpretation supposes that the mother not only modified the body of her unborn child with her thoughts, but after the child's birth influenced it to make statements and show behavior that it otherwise would not have done. No motive for such conduct can be discerned in most of the mothers (or fathers) of these subjects.It is not my purpose to impose any interpretation of these cases on the readers of this article. Nor would I expect any reader to reach even a preliminary conclusion from the short summaries of cases that the brevity of this report entails.
Instead, I hope that I have
stimulated readers to examine the detailed reports of many cases that I am now
in the process of publishing (Stevenson, forthcoming).
"Originality and truth are found only in the details" (Stendhal, 1926).
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I am grateful to Drs. Antonia Mills and Emily W. Cook for critical comments on drafts of this paper. Thanks are also due to the Bernstein Brothers Parapsychology and Health Foundation for the support of my research.
Correspondence and requests for reprints should be addressed to: Ian Stevenson, M.D., Division of Perceptual Studies, Box 152, Health Sciences Center, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22908
I am grateful to Drs. Antonia Mills and Emily W. Cook for critical comments on drafts of this paper. Thanks are also due to the Bernstein Brothers Parapsychology and Health Foundation for the support of my research.
Correspondence and requests for reprints should be addressed to: Ian Stevenson, M.D., Division of Perceptual Studies, Box 152, Health Sciences Center, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22908
REFERENCES
Cockayne, E, A. (1933). Inherited
abnormalities of the skin. London: Oxford University Press.
Cook, E. W., Pasricha, S, Samararatne, G, Win Maung, & Stevenson, I. (1983). Review and analysis of "unsolved" cases of the reincarnation type: II. Comparison of features of solved and unsolved cases, Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 77, 1 15-135.
Dabney, W. C. (1890). Maternal impressions. In J. M. Keating (Ed.), Cyclopaedia of the diseases of children, Vol. 1 , (pp. 191-216). Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott.
Denaro, S. J. ( 1944). The inheritance of nevi. Journal of Heredity, 35, 2 1 5- 1 8.
Fatteh, A. (1976). Medicolegal investigation ofgunshor wounds. Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott.
Frantz, C. H., & O'Rahilly, R.(1961). Congenital skeletal limb deficiencies. Journal ofBone and Joins Surgerq: 43-A, 1202-24.
Gordon, I., & Shapiro, H. A. (1982). Forensic medicine: A guide to principles. (2nd ed.) London: Churchill Livingstone.
Hill, A, B,, Doll, R,, Galloway, T. M., & Hughes, J.P.W. (1958). Virus diseases in pregnancy and congenital defects. British Journal of Preventive and Social Medicine, 12, 1-7.
Maruri, C. A. (1961). La herencia en dermarologia. (2nd ed.) Santander: Aldus, S.A. Artes Graficas.
Mills, A. (1989). A replication study: Three cases of children in northern India who are said to remember a previous life. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 3, 133-184.
Nelson, K., & Holmes, L. B. (1989). Malformations due to presumed spontaneous mutations in newborn infants. New England Journal ofMedicine, 320, 19-23.
Nowack, E, (1965). Die sensible Phase bei der Thalidomid- Embryopathie. Humangenetik, I, 516-36.
Pack, G. T., & Davis, J. (1956). Moles. New York Stare Journal ofMedicine, 56, 3498-3506.
Pack, G. T., Lenson, N. & Gerber, D. M. (1952). Regional distribution of moles and melanomas. AMA Archives ofSurgery. 65, 862-70.
Smith, D. W. (1982). Recognizable patterns of human malformation. (3rd ed.) Philadelphia: W. BSaunders.
Spalteholz. W (1943). Hand atlas of human anatomy. Translated by L. E Barker. 7th English ed. Philadelphia: J,B. Lippincott.
Stendhal (1926). Lucien Leuwen. Paris: Librairie Ancienne Honor6 Champion, 4, 169.
Stevenson, I. (1975). Cases of the reincarnation type. I. Ten cases in India. CharlottesviIle: University Press of Virginia.
Stevenson, I. (1983). American children who claim to remember previous lives. Journal ofNervous and Mental Disease, 17 1, 742-748.
Stevenson, I. (1987). Children who remember previous lives. Charlottesville: University Press of Virginia.
Stevenson, I. ( 1990). Phobias in children who claim to remember previous lives. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 4, 243-254.
Stevenson, I. (1992). A new look at maternal impressions: An analysis of 50 published cases and reports of two recent examples. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 6, 353-373.
Stevenson, I. (Forthcoming). Birthmarks and birth defects: A contribution to their etiology.
Williams, H. C., & Pembroke, A. C. (1988). Naevus of Jamaica. Lancer, 11, 915.
Wilson, J. G. (1973). Environment and birth defects. New York: Academic Press.
Cook, E. W., Pasricha, S, Samararatne, G, Win Maung, & Stevenson, I. (1983). Review and analysis of "unsolved" cases of the reincarnation type: II. Comparison of features of solved and unsolved cases, Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 77, 1 15-135.
Dabney, W. C. (1890). Maternal impressions. In J. M. Keating (Ed.), Cyclopaedia of the diseases of children, Vol. 1 , (pp. 191-216). Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott.
Denaro, S. J. ( 1944). The inheritance of nevi. Journal of Heredity, 35, 2 1 5- 1 8.
Fatteh, A. (1976). Medicolegal investigation ofgunshor wounds. Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott.
Frantz, C. H., & O'Rahilly, R.(1961). Congenital skeletal limb deficiencies. Journal ofBone and Joins Surgerq: 43-A, 1202-24.
Gordon, I., & Shapiro, H. A. (1982). Forensic medicine: A guide to principles. (2nd ed.) London: Churchill Livingstone.
Hill, A, B,, Doll, R,, Galloway, T. M., & Hughes, J.P.W. (1958). Virus diseases in pregnancy and congenital defects. British Journal of Preventive and Social Medicine, 12, 1-7.
Maruri, C. A. (1961). La herencia en dermarologia. (2nd ed.) Santander: Aldus, S.A. Artes Graficas.
Mills, A. (1989). A replication study: Three cases of children in northern India who are said to remember a previous life. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 3, 133-184.
Nelson, K., & Holmes, L. B. (1989). Malformations due to presumed spontaneous mutations in newborn infants. New England Journal ofMedicine, 320, 19-23.
Nowack, E, (1965). Die sensible Phase bei der Thalidomid- Embryopathie. Humangenetik, I, 516-36.
Pack, G. T., & Davis, J. (1956). Moles. New York Stare Journal ofMedicine, 56, 3498-3506.
Pack, G. T., Lenson, N. & Gerber, D. M. (1952). Regional distribution of moles and melanomas. AMA Archives ofSurgery. 65, 862-70.
Smith, D. W. (1982). Recognizable patterns of human malformation. (3rd ed.) Philadelphia: W. BSaunders.
Spalteholz. W (1943). Hand atlas of human anatomy. Translated by L. E Barker. 7th English ed. Philadelphia: J,B. Lippincott.
Stendhal (1926). Lucien Leuwen. Paris: Librairie Ancienne Honor6 Champion, 4, 169.
Stevenson, I. (1975). Cases of the reincarnation type. I. Ten cases in India. CharlottesviIle: University Press of Virginia.
Stevenson, I. (1983). American children who claim to remember previous lives. Journal ofNervous and Mental Disease, 17 1, 742-748.
Stevenson, I. (1987). Children who remember previous lives. Charlottesville: University Press of Virginia.
Stevenson, I. ( 1990). Phobias in children who claim to remember previous lives. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 4, 243-254.
Stevenson, I. (1992). A new look at maternal impressions: An analysis of 50 published cases and reports of two recent examples. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 6, 353-373.
Stevenson, I. (Forthcoming). Birthmarks and birth defects: A contribution to their etiology.
Williams, H. C., & Pembroke, A. C. (1988). Naevus of Jamaica. Lancer, 11, 915.
Wilson, J. G. (1973). Environment and birth defects. New York: Academic Press.
“As the moon dies and comes to life
again,
so we also, having to die,
will rise again." ~ San Juan
Capistrano Indians
interesting ...
ReplyDeletea theory:
every soul must re-live any and all pains it causes to all souls (including human, animal, any living beings (all of which have a soul)
So to determine length of time of this process for likes of the politicos, war financiers, war profiteers, criminals, mass-communications liars, slavers, etc.
multiply the amount of human souls present now (and future who will be effected by the present times activities)
add to it the souls contained in nature (from plants, trees, animals, to fishes, birds, insects, etc.)
and this is amount of time (trillions of ages) these doomed souls will pass through before (if) they arrive at new start over ...(that is "hell" - some of these sick souls never are allowed to leave)
for this reader - to do anything, everything positive possible is the goal,
so as to never again return to this "hell dimension" again
maybe another dimension or world, but not this one again